Nature or nurture? The Words You Hear Make You Smarter.

Were Albert Einstein and Leonardo da Vinci born brilliant
or did they acquire their intelligence through effort?

The authors of this report point out that their research"does not weigh in on the age-old "nature vs. nurture" debate."

Yet to my reading it does, and comes down on both sides of the issue.  Basically, this research states that having smarts and talent can be boosted by the words you.  Now, this isn't exactly news.  Research released last year pointed out that five minutes of relaxation during which you replay past successes actually increases your I.Q. by about 10 points.

So you can increase the power of the tools and abilities nature has given you by a simple relaxation technique combined with positive self-imaging.

The power of words, whether your own internal conversation or what others tell you, has a powerful impact on how you are able to express your natural gifts.  Here's a story that explains this.


Simply telling people that hard work is more important than genetics causes positive changes in the brain and may make them willing to try harder, a study shows. "Giving people messages that encourage learning and motivation may promote more efficient performance," said the lead investigator. "In contrast, telling people that intelligence is genetically fixed may inadvertently hamper learning."

Were Albert Einstein and Leonardo da Vinci born brilliant or did they acquire their intelligence through effort?

No one knows for sure, but telling people the latter -- that hard work trumps genes -- causes instant changes in the brain and may make them more willing to strive for success, indicates a new study from Michigan State University.

The findings suggest the human brain is more receptive to the message that intelligence comes from the environment, regardless of whether it's true. And this simple message, said lead investigator Hans Schroder, may ultimately prompt us to work harder.

Suggested reading
click on image
"Giving people messages that encourage learning and motivation may promote more efficient performance," said Schroder, a doctoral student in clinical psychology whose work is funded by the National Science Foundation. "In contrast, telling people that intelligence is genetically fixed may inadvertently hamper learning."

In past research by Stanford University psychologist Carol Dweck, elementary students performing a task were either praised for their intelligence ("You're so smart!") or for their effort ("You worked really hard!") after correct responses. As the task became harder, children in the first group performed worse after their mistakes compared to the group that had heard effort was important.

The MSU study, which appears online in the journal Biological Psychology, offers what could be the first physiological evidence to support those findings, in the form of a positive brain response. "These subtle messages seem to have a big impact, and now we can see they have an immediate impact on how the brain handles information about performance," Schroder said.

For the study, two groups of participants read different articles. One article reported that intelligence is largely genetic, while the other said the brilliance of da Vinci and Einstein was "probably due to a challenging environment. Their genius had little to do with genetic structure."
Participants were instructed to remember the main points of the article and completed a simple computer task while their brain activity was recorded. The findings, in a nutshell:

The group that read intelligence was primarily genetic paid more attention to their responses, as if they were more concerned with their performance. This extra attention, however, did not relate to performance on trials after errors, suggesting a disconnect between brain and behavior.
In contrast, those who had read that intelligence was due to a challenging environment showed a more efficient brain response after they made a mistake, possibly because they believed they could do better on the next trial. The more attention these participants paid to mistakes, the faster their responses were on the next trial.

The study does not weigh in on the age-old "nature vs. nurture" debate, Schroder noted. Rather, it investigates the messages about the nature of abilities people are exposed to on a regular basis, from a teacher comforting a student ("It's OK, not everyone can be a math person") to the sports announcer commenting on a player's skill ("Wow, what a natural!"). These messages are thought to contribute to the attitudes or "mindsets" people hold about their intelligence and abilities.

Related posts:



*  *  *  *  *
Story Source:  Materials provided by Michigan State University. Hans S. Schroder, Tim P. Moran, M. Brent Donnellan, Jason S. Moser. Mindset induction effects on cognitive control: A neurobehavioral investigation. Biological Psychology, 2014

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Workout supplements: An emerging eating disorder in men?

Remember faces but not names? You got it wrong.

Children's drawings during abuse investigations

Unhappy in a relationship? Why do you stay?

M.I.T. Research Explains How Will Rogers Observation Works