Why conformity can be a good thing


Majority rule: Why conformity 
can actually be a good thing

"Our whole world is made up of things that we 
do that are good for us, but we don't know why." 

Like to go your own way? Most of us actually prefer to follow the pack, according to UBC research.

That's one of the outcomes from a study published in Evolution and Human Behavior that examines how mathematical models predict human behavior.

The research tested theories about when people should rely on "social information" -- information that we learn vicariously from others -- and when we should choose to go it alone.

Nurturing
Independent Thinkers


 by Michael Bosher

Order new or used from
Powell's Books
"People are conformist -- and that's a good thing for cultural evolution," said Michael Muthukrishna, a Vanier and Liu Scholar and recent PhD recipient from UBC's department of psychology. "By being conformist, we copy the things that are popular in the world. And those things are often good and useful."

For example, most people don't understand how germs can cause disease -- but they know they should wash their hands after using the bathroom. "Our whole world is made up of things that we do that are good for us, but we don't know why," said Muthukrishna. "And we don't need to know why. We just need to know that most people do those things."

The research also found that people with higher IQs don't follow the pack as much as others -- but when they do, they do so more strategically. In other words, smarter people tend to take their own path most of the time, because they think they have the correct answer. When they're unsure, however, they are more willing than those with average IQs to follow the majority.

In addition, the more diversity there is in behavior, the more likely people are to copy the majority. As the number of available options increases, this creates more uncertainty -- and in such circumstances, a majority sends an even stronger signal.

"These mathematical theories and experiments contribute to a greater understanding of what it is that makes our species so unique -- culture," said Muthukrishna. "Our smarts are acquired, not hardwired."

BACKGROUND
The study, "The when and who of social learning and conformist transmission," is published in Evolution and Human Behavior. Muthukrishna's co-authors are Thomas Morgan from the University of California, Berkeley and Joseph Henrich from UBC and the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research.

The study involved 101 participants, recruited from the UBC student population and the general population. They took part in two experiments; both involved a perceptual task comparing differing amounts of lines, with the purpose of identifying the longest line. In both experiments, participants were made aware of others' choices; the second experiment also featured random computer-generated answers. Both experiments included pay incentives, which were based on performance.

Related stories:
Story Source:  Materials provided by University of British Columbia.  Michael Muthukrishna, Thomas J.H. Morgan, Joseph Henrich. The when and who of social learning and conformist transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 2015

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Workout supplements: An emerging eating disorder in men?

Remember faces but not names? You got it wrong.

Children's drawings during abuse investigations

Unhappy in a relationship? Why do you stay?

M.I.T. Research Explains How Will Rogers Observation Works